Sank's Glossary of Linguistics
Np-Nz |
NPI
See NEGATIVE POLARITY ITEM.
NUCLEAR SYLLABLE
(Prosody) A syllable that carries maximum prominence, usually due to being stressed.
- I will give you a permit.
(Stress is on the nuclear syllable per of permit.)
- I will permit you to do it.
(Stress is on the nuclear syllable mit of permit.)
| Alphabetical Glossary of Linguistic Terms, ?
NUCLEAR TONE
(Prosody) In the normal, unemphatic version of the sentence The lady saw the dog, the last word is nuclear, and will carry one of the possible nuclear tones in English. The nuclear tone is the most prominent pitch movement in a tone unit. In English, analyses of nuclear tones vary, but most recognize such contrasts as falling, rising, rising-falling, falling-rising, and level. Others are possible, including distinctions within these types, such as high vs. low fall. | David Crystal, 2008
NUCLEUS
- (Syntax) A (predicate) nucleus is a unit consisting of the syntactic predicate and its arguments (excluding adjuncts). | I Wayan Arka, 2003
- (Prosody; Phonology) Or, tonic syllable. Usually used in the description of intonation to refer to the most prominent syllable of the
tone-unit, but also used in phonology to denote the center or peak (i.e. vowel or syllabic consonant) of a syllable. It is one of the central principles of the "standard British" treatment of intonation that continuous speech can be broken up into units called tone-units, and that each of these will have one syllable that can be identified as the most prominent. This syllable will normally be the starting point of the major pitch movement (nuclear tone) in the tone-unit. | Peter Roach, 2011
- (Phonology) A rhyme is a term used to describe the unit of a syllable which contains both the nucleus and the coda. The nucleus of the syllable is the most sonorous part of a syllable and is obligatory in all languages.
- /səpíq/ 'yellow salmonberry'
Foot
/ \
/ \
σ σ
/| /|\
/ | / μ \
/ | / | \
(s ə p í q)
The first syllable (σ) does not contain a coda and is characterized by only the onset and the nucleus being parsed into a single syllable (/sə/). Following, there is a second syllable which contains an onset (/p/) and a rhyme (nucleus /í/ + coda /q/), and it can be seen that all three segments are parsed into the second syllable (Dyck 2004). | INLP Linguistic Glossary, 2021
NULL ARGUMENT
- (Syntax) Or, missing argument, or, argument ellipsis. Refers to the omission from a clause of one or more of three types of nominals required by the main verb: the surface Subject, the Direct Object, and/or the Indirect Object. | Sigal Uziel-Karl, 2013
- (Example) Korean allows null arguments, as shown in (1), unlike English, as shown in (2).
A:
na-nun
I-NOM
John-uy
J.-GEN
hyeng-ul
brother-ACC
poa / manna-ss-ta.
see/meet-PST-DEC
'I saw / met John's brother.'
B:
na-to
I-too
∅
∅
poa/manna-ss-ta.
see / meet-PST-DEC
Lit. 'I also saw / met.'
- A: I saw/met John's brother.
B: I also saw/met *(him).
| Hee-Don Ahn and Sungeun Cho, 2012
NULL BINDING
(Example) If, as in (1a), the topic is understood as a class or type, then it can bind an empty predicate complement in the comment clause—but not if, as in (1b), it is understood as an individual. In the latter case, however, if the bound element is lexicalized, the result is grammatical:
a.
Tā
he
shì
be
gè
a
xiǎotōu,
thief
qiángdào
robber
tā
he
hái
still
bù
not
shì
be
'He is a thief, but a robber he isn't.'
b.
* Lǐ
Qìnyú
wǒ
I
zhǐ
refer
de
MOD
jìu
precisely
shì.
be
* 'Li Qinyu, the one I refer to is precisely.'
c.
Lǐ
Qìnyú
wǒ
I
zhǐ
refer
de
MOD
jíu
precisely
shí
be
tā.
he
'Li Qinyu, the one I refer to is precisely he.'
d.
Lǐ
Qìnyú
wǒ
I
zhǐ
refer
de
MOD
jìu
precisely
shì
be
zhège
this
rén.
man
'Li Qinyu, the one I refer to is precisely this man.'
| Xu Liejiong and D. Terence Langendoen, 1985
NULL COMPLEMENT ANAPHORA
- (Syntax) We observe that certain verbs in Spanish and Italian allow their infinitival / clausal complements to be null. However, sentences containing them become ungrammatical when we try to extract an element that would have appeared inside the clausal complement in the non-null version, such as a clitic, a wh-phrase, etc., ... or when restructuring has occurred. We propose and provide evidence that these null clausal complements in Spanish and Italian are instances of Null Complement Anaphora, a type of deep anaphor in Hankamer and Sag's (1976) sense and not an instance of surface anaphor such as VP ellipsis in English. | Marcela A. Depiante, 2001
- (Syntax) Hankamer and Sag (1976) and Sag (1980) distinguish the constructions of NCA and VP ellipsis in English, on the basis of empirical evidence, and they claim that NCA, in opposition to VP ellipsis, is a deep anaphor.
In NCA, (1), the null constituent presents either sentential or predicative content, whereas in VP Ellipsis, (2), the elided constituent corresponds only to the predicate:
- a. I asked Bill to leave, but he refused e.
(e = to leave) (H&S 1976)
b. He said one of us had to give up his seat, so Sue volunteered e.
(e = to give up her seat) (H&S 1976)
- I repeatedly asked Bill to leave, and he finally did e.
(e = leave)
Also, in NCA the null constituent is licensed by a main verb, (1); while in VP Ellipsis in English an auxiliary or to infinitive licenses the gap, cf. (2) and (3).
- He said one of us had to give up his seat, so Sue volunteered to e.
(e = give up her seat) (Sag 1980)
Besides, NCA contrasts with VP ellipsis because in the former construction, the null constituent may be recovered by a pragmatic context (4), while in the latter the ellipsis must be recovered by a linguistic antecedent, (5).
- [Situation: Indulgent father feeds baby chocolate bar for dinner.]
Mother: I don't approve e !
(e = that you feed him chocolate bar for dinner) (H&S 1976)
- [Situation: Hankamer attempts to stuff a 9-inch ball through a 6-inch hoop.]
Sag: # It's not clear that you'll be able to e.
(H&S 1976)
In addition, in NCA the null constituent may denote a linguistic antecedent structurally different from the one selected by the licensing verb. In VP Ellipsis, however, the linguistic antecedent must be lexically and structurally parallel to the elided constituent. Thus, in (6), where the omitted constituent must be recovered in the active voice although its antecedent is in the passive,
NCA is grammatical, (6a), but VP ellipsis is marginal, (6b).
- The oats had to be taken down to the bin,
a. so Bill volunteered e.
(e = to take the oats down to the bin)
b. * so Bill did e.
(e = take the oats down to the bin) (H&S 1976)
Finally, the null constituent in NCA does not present internal structure; hence, it does not sanction the Missing Antecedent construction (Grinder and Postal 1971), while it does in VP ellipsis: it in (7a) does not have an antecedent in the complex sentence, contrary to what happens in (7b):
- a. * He said one of us had to give up his seat, so Sue volunteered e, because it was too narrow for her anyway. (H&S 1976)
b. He said one of us had to give up his seat, so Sue did e, because it was too narrow for her anyway. (H&S 1976)
| Sonia Cyrino and Gabriela Mato, 2004
NULL OPERATOR
(Syntax) This dissertation defends the hypothesis that certain constructions involve covert A′-movement resulting in the creation of A′-chains headed by null categories commonly referred to as null operators. The constructions which have been widely analyzed as involving null operators fall into two categories.
- OWM ('On wh-movement') constructions are relative clauses (tensed and infinitival), purpose clauses, degree clauses, adjectival complements, clefts and comparatives, all of which are treated as covert wh-movement structures in Chomsky (1977).
- More recently, the null operator hypothesis has been extended to parasitic gap constructions.
A′-chains headed by empty categories first appear in Chomsky (1980), where it is argued that, in certain constructions, base-generated PRO moves to COMP, creating an operator-variable chain analogous to that created by overt wh-operator movement. The impetus for positing the existence of empty category-headed A′-chains originates in the
proposal in Chomsky (1973) that relative clauses such as (1) are derived by deletion of an overt wh-operator which has previously been moved to COMP, as shown in (2).
- the person Bill saw
- a. [NP the person [S′ [COMP who] [S Bill saw ] ] ]
b. [NP the person [O′ [COMP ] [S Bill saw ] ] ]
| Marguerite Browning, 1987
See Also PARASITIC GAP.
NULL-SUBJECT
(Syntax) Or, pro-drop. One of the parameters of universal grammar. The concept arises from the permissible dropping, in some languages, of the subject pronoun of a sentence because of potential pragmatic recoverability from context. The content of null-subject is phonologically and morphologically covert but is recoverable in context by competent native speakers of the language. A null-subject is said to have grammatical and semantic properties but lacks overt phonetic form. Going by the parameter of null-subject, some languages are classified as null-subject or pro-drop languages while some are classified as non-null-subject or non-pro-drop languages. English, according to Chomsky (1995) and Radford (2004) is a non-pro-drop language, but Italian is a pro-drop language because it allows finite verbs to have null-subject in its syntax. | Odingowei Kwokwo Macdonald, 2016
See Also PRO-DROP.
NULL SUBJECT LANGUAGE
- (Syntax) As is well-known, Spanish is a language in which person and number information is explicitly realized in verbal morphology. Typologically, we say that the language is a consistent null subject language, not only because pronominal subjects are productively dropped, as in (2), but also because lexical subjects freely invert with respect to the position of the verb, as in (1) and (3).
Nosotros
we
cantamos.
sing.1PL
'We sing.'
-
Cantamos
sing.1PL
nosotros.
we
| Andrés Saab, 2023
- (Syntax) Greek (2), by contrast to English (1), is a null-subject language, hence, subject pronouns most often remain without phonological content.
- * (She) loves Kostas.
agapa
love.3S
ton
the.ACC
Kosta
Kostas.ACC
'(She) loves Kostas.'
| Arhonto Terzi, Theodoros Marinis, Anthi Zafeiri, and Konstantinos Francis, 2019
NULL SUBJECT PARAMETER
- (Syntax)
The Null Subject Parameter
Null subjects are permitted in all and only languages with morphologically uniform inflectional paradigms.
| Osvaldo Jaeggli and Kenneth J. Safir, 1989
- (Syntax) The NSP can be plausibly conceived of as the theory of all predictable aspects of grammatical variation in the realm of subject realization across languages. A strong intuition is that there is a correlation between agreement richness in verbal morphology and the possibility of subject elision (Taraldsen 1978).
Null Subject Parameter
case/agreement dependency between T0 and the subject?
╱╲
╱ ╲
[no: Japanese, Chinese] [yes: dissociated agreement?]
╱╲
╱ ╲
[no: English, Standard German] [yes: generalized person dissociation?]
╱╲
╱ ╲
[no: Brazilian Portuguese] [yes: subject clitics?]
╱╲
╱ ╲
[no: Spanish, European Portuguese] [yes: Trentino]
| Andrés Saab, 2023
See Also PRO-DROP PARAMETER.
NUMBER AGREEMENT
- (Grammar) Agreement in number between words in the same construction, e.g., between adjectives and the nouns they modify. | Free Dictionary, 2017
- (Grammar) Agreement based on grammatical number can occur between verb and subject, as in the case of grammatical person. In fact the two categories are often conflated within verb conjugation patterns: there are specific verb forms for first person singular, second person plural and so on. Some examples:
- I really am (1st pers. singular) vs. We really are (1st pers. plural)
- The boy sings (3rd pers. singular) vs. The boys sing (3rd pers. plural)
Again as with person, there is agreement in number between pronouns (or their corresponding possessives) and antecedents:
- The girl did her job vs. The girls did their job
Agreement also occurs between nouns and their specifier and modifiers, in some situations. This is common in languages such as French and Spanish, where articles, determiners and adjectives (both attributive and predicative) agree in number with the nouns they qualify:
- le grand homme ('the great man') vs. les grands hommes ('the great men')
- el hombre alto ('the tall man') vs. los hombres altos ('the tall men')
In English this is not such a common feature, although there are certain determiners that occur specifically with singular or plural nouns only:
- One big car vs. Two big cars
- Much great work vs. Many great works
| Wikipedia, 2023
NUMBER PHRASE
- (Syntax) Abbreviated NumP. A functional projection between D and NP, which we call Number Phrase. In a full noun phrase, the head of this projection is, among other things, the locus of number specification (singular or plural) of a noun phrase. Pronominal noun phrases are distinguished from full noun phrases by the fact that they lack a lexical projection, i.e. they lack a NP. The existence of two distinct functional categories predicts the existence of at least two classes of pronouns,
those of the category D, and those of the category Num. In both Modern Hebrew and Haitian, there is evidence that this prediction is borne out. | Elizabeth Ritter, 1992
- (Syntax) I will motivate the following hierarchical representation that, moving top-down, consists of a Determiner Phrase (DP), a Cardinal Number Phrase (CardP), an Agreement Phrase (AgrP = Julien's αP), the proposed article Phrase (ArtP), a Number Phrase (NumP), a light noun Phrase (nP) and NP:
[DP D [CardP Card [AgrP Agr [ArtP Art [ NumP Num [ nP n [NP N ] ] ] ] ] ] ]
| Dorian Roehrs, 2009
NUMERATION
- (Syntax) A formal device used in the Minimalist Program. A numeration is a set that contains pairs of
- the lexical items that will be used in a syntactic derivation and
- an index that indicates how often each lexical item will be used in the derivation.
E.g., (2) is the numeration for the sentence in (1):
- The women built the airplanes.
- {<the,2>, <women,1>, <built,1>, <airplanes,1>, <T(ense),1>}
Chomsky (1995) argues that derivations using the same numeration constitute the reference set for a derivation; these derivations are candidates for comparison with respect to economy conditions, and only the "cheapest" candidate, i.e. the one that violates the least economy conditions, is grammatical. With the arisal of local economy, the usefulness of numerations is less clear. | Utrecht Lexicon of Linguistics 2001
- (Syntax) In current minimalist theory, a linguistic expression is a pair (π, λ) consisting of a PF representation (i.e., sound) associated to an LF representation (i.e., a meaning). Under this view, the role of the computational system CHL is to map some array of lexical choices to the pair (π, λ). The array of lexical choices is referred to as the "numeration" (N), which Chomsky (1995) defines as:
A set of pairs (LI, i ), where LI is an item of the lexicon and i is its index, understood to be the number of times that LI is selected.
Each time a lexical item is selected from the numeration (N), its index is reduced by one so that the converging derivation (i.e., the one that forms a linguistic expression) is the one for which N is reduced to zero. | Enoch O. Aboh, 2010
Page Last Modified March 26, 2025