Sank's Glossary of Linguistics 
Clause-Cn

CLAUSE BOUNDARY IDENTIFICATION
(Computational) A useful technique for various Natural Language Processing (NLP) applications. This is a method of specifying the beginning and ending of main and subordinate clauses. Numerous techniques are used to recognize clause boundaries for different languages where some are rule-based (Harris 1997, Vilson 1998) and others are statistical approaches using machine learning techniques (Vijay and Sobha, 2008). | Daraksha Parveen, Ratna Sanyal, and Afreen Ansari, 2011

CLAUSE-EMBEDDING PREDICATE

  1. (Syntax) A verb like know and guess in English, which can be immediately followed by a clause as in I know that it's raining out. | Jennifer Culbertson and Wataru Uegaki, 2022
  2. (Syntax) "Clause-embedding predicates", like know and believe, are a central case in the study of projection. | Marie-Catherine de Marneffe, Mandy Simons, and Judith Tonhauser, 2018
  3. (Syntax; Semantics) Recent work has explored semantic universals in the domain of "clause-embedding predicates" like know, agree, and wonder (Spector and Egré 2015; Theiler, Roelofsen and Aloni 2018; Uegaki 2019; Steinert-Threlkeld 2020). | Floris Roelofsen and Wataru Uegaki, 2020
  4. (Syntax; Pragmatics) When a speaker uses a factive "clause-embedding predicate" like know in (1) in a conversation, the listener can infer that the speaker is committed to the truth of the proposition p: that Julian dances salsa.
    1. John knows that Julian dances salsa.
     | Dingyi Pan and Judith Degen, 2023

CLAUSEMATE CONDITION
(Syntax) 

The Clausemate Condition (CMC) on Multiple Sluicing (MS) (Takahashi 1994)
The wh-remnants in a MS construction must originate in the same (finite) clause.
 It has been reported to apply cross-linguistically.
  1. English (Lasnik 2014):
    1. [CP1 Fred thinks [CP2 that a certain boy talked to a certain girl]].
      I wish I could remember which boy to which girl.
    2. * [CP1 A certain boy said [CP2 that Fred talked to a certain girl]].
      I wish I could remember which boy to which girl.
  2. Spanish (Rodrigues et al. 2009):
    1. [CP1
      Juan
      Juan
      dijo
      said
      que
      that
      [CP2
      unos
      some
      estudiantes
      students
      van
      will
      a
      hablar
      talk
      con
      with
      un
      a
      profesor]],
      professor
      pero
      but
      no
      not
      recuerdo
      remember
      qué
      what
      estudiante
      students
      con
      with
      qué
      what
      professor.
      professor
    2. * [CP1
      Unos
      some
      estudiantes
      students
      dijeron
      said
      que
      that
      [CP2
      Juan
      John
      va
      will
      a
      hablar
      talk
      con
      with
      un
      a
      profesor]],
      professor
      pero
      but
      no
      not
      recuerdo
      remember
      qué
      what
      estudiante
      students
      con
      with
      qué
      what
      profesor.
      professor

     | Álvaro Cortés Rodríguez, 2022

CLEFT

  1. (Syntax) In cleft sentences the copula is preceded by it and followed by a NP and a relative clause. | ?
  2. (Syntax) Cleft sentences are composed of four structural subparts:
    1. cleft pronoun + copula + clefted constituent + cleft clause
      it + is + the woman + who buys the house
     | Nancy Hedberg, 2008
  3. (Syntax) At least three different analyses have been proposed for the so-called "Cleft-sentence" construction in English. The construction type is well illustrated by the following example:
    1. It is the wife who decides.
     | Robert B. Lees, 1963
  4. (Syntax) Most linguists who concern themselves with the structure of English would be familiar with the term "cleft sentence", and would use the term to designate expressions like (1).
    1. It was the butler who served the wine.
     Others might include in this category sentences like (2).
    1. The one who served the wine was the butler.
     Yet a few others might regard sentences such as (3) as cleft sentences, too.
    1. The butler was the one who served the wine.
     The first linguist in the 20th century to draw attention to these sentence patterns was Otto Jespersen. He described the IT-clefts, e.g., sentence (1) above, in some detail in his Modern English Grammar (1965). To be sure, he did not call them "cleft sentences" there. However, in 1937, the term appears prominently in his Analytic Syntax (1969), along with some speculations about how this construction arose. As he saw it, the phrase it is plus the connective, if present, is superimposed upon the simple sentence, which is the semantic substance of the cleft. Thus, the expression (4) results from the insertion of the clefting elements it is ... who into the simple sentence (5).
    1. It is the wife who decides.
    2. The wife decides.
     | Edward G. Fichtner, 2015

CLITIC
(Morphology; Syntax) Element which, like affixes, cannot occur freely in syntax but is in need of a host. A "clitic" can thus be regarded as a kind of bound morpheme. A typical clitic will attach itself to a host, that is, a (fully inflected) word or phrase. The observation that they can attach to inflected words distinguishes, among other things, clitics from affixes. Clitics come in two types: proclitics and enclitics. Proclitics attach themselves to the left of the host, while enclitics attach themselves to the right of the host.
 Example: In French, object pronouns are clitics which are either proclitics, as me and les in (1), or enclitics, as les in (2):

  1. il
    he
    me
    to-me
    les
    them
    a
    has
    donnés
    given
    'he has given them to me'
  2. donnez-les-moi
    give-them-me
    'give them to me'
  3. il mei lesj a donnés ei ej
 In syntax it is usually assumed that a clitic is related to a gap, an empty category (trace or pro). Example (1) is analyzed as in (3), where e is a gap. (Haegeman 1991; Kayne 1990, 1975; Klavans 1985, 1982; Nespor and Vogel 1986; Rizzi 1986; Spencer 1991; Zwicky 1977; Zwicky and Pullum 1983) | Utrecht Lexicon of Linguistics, 2001

CLITIC DOUBLING
(Syntax) The use of (mostly object) clitic pronouns together with a coreferential noun phrase, especially in the Romance and Balkan languages.

  1. Romanian
    L-am
    3SG.M.CL-I.have
    văzut
    seen
    pe
    ACC
    Petru.
    Petru
    'I've seen Petru.'
  2. Macedonian
    Jana
    Jana
    go
    3SG.M.ACC.CL
    vide
    saw
    volk-ot.
    wolf-DEF
    'Jana saw the wolf.'
 | Glottopedia, 2008

CLITIC LEFT DISLOCATION

  1. (Syntax) The work of Cinque (1990) on Italian, and Iatridou (1995) and Anagnostopoulou (1994) on Greek, has demonstrated the possibility of a left-peripheral XP, apparently in an A'-position, and associated with a clitic, but which is not a case of left-dislocation of the English kind. This is the construction known as "Clitic Left Dislocation" (CLLD).
     CLLD has been exempli ed in the literature in various languages:
    1. Italian
      In
      in
      quella
      that
      città,
      town
      non
      not
      ci
      there
      sono
      am
      mai
      ever
      stato.
      been
      'I have never been in that town.'
    2. French
      Au
      to-the
      pape,
      pope
      personne
      no-one
      n'oserait
      NEG-would-dare
      lui
      to-him
      parler
      speak
      ainsi.
      thus
      'No one would dare to speak to the pope like that.'
    3. Greek
      Ta
      the
      klidia
      keys
      ta
      them
      stilame
      sent
      sti
      to-the
      Maria.
      Maria
      'We sent the keys to Maria.'
    4. Lebanese Arabic
      fakkart
      thought
      ʔinno
      that
      Naadya
      Nadia
      Seef-a
      saw-her
      Kariim
      Kerim
      mbeerih.
      yesterday
      'I thought that Kerim saw Nadia yesterday.'
     Some characteristics:  | Theodora Alexopoulou, Edit Doron and Caroline Heycock, 2006
  2. (Syntax) Clitic Left Dislocated NPs in Greek and certain Romance languages (Italian, Catalan, Romanian) have long been associated with a given, discourse-linked or topic interpretation (Philippaki-Warburton 1985, Cinque 1990, Dobrovie-Sorin 1990, Anagnostopoulou 1994, Schneider-Zioga 1994, Iatridou 1995, Tsimpli 1995). More recently, they were analyzed as links (Vallduví 1992, Vallduví 1995 for Catalan; Alexopoulou 1999 for Greek).
    1. Pii
      simetehoun
      s'
      afti
      tin
      paragogi?
      'Who contributed to this production?'
      Tin
      the
      parastasi
      performance-FEM.ACC
      ti
      FEM.3SG.ACC
      skinothetise
      directed-3SG
      o
      the
      Karolos
      Karolos-NOM
      Koun.
      Koun
      'Karolos Koun directed the performance.'
     | Theodora Alexopoulou and Dimitra Kolliakou, 2001

CLITICIZATION
(Morphosyntax) A process by which a complex word is formed by attaching a clitic to a fully inflected word.

  1. In Je t'aime (French, 'I love you'), the clitic t' ('you') is attached to the verb aime ('love').
 It is unclear whether cliticization is a type of word formation or a type of syntactic operation (incorporation) (Zwicky 1977, et al). | Utrecht Lexicon of Linguistics, 2001

CLOSED SYLLABLE SHORTENING

  1. (Phonology) When underlying long vowels shorten in closed syllables, as in (1).
    1. Pāli
      /jaː-nta/ → [jan.ta]
      go-PRESENT PARTICIPLE
     | Paul de Lacy, 2006
  2. (Phonology) Another shortening process in Old English was closed syllable shortening (CSS) (Luick, 1914/1964). According to Luick, at the early stages CSS occurred when a long vowel was followed by three consonants, as in brĕmblas from * brēmblas 'brambles'. Later, CSS also applied if the long vowel was followed by two consonants as in ĕnlefan from * ǣnlefan 'eleven'. | Aditi Lahiri and Paula Fikkert, 1999

CLUSIVITY
(Grammar) A distinction (Filimonova 2005) between inclusive and exclusive first-person pronouns and verbal morphology, also called inclusive "we" and exclusive "we". Inclusive "we" specifically includes the addressee (that is, one of the words for 'we' means 'you and I and possibly others'), while exclusive "we" specifically excludes the addressee (that is, another word for 'we' means 'he/she/they and I, but not you'), regardless of who else may be involved.
 While imagining that this sort of distinction could be made in other persons (particularly the second) is straightforward, in fact the existence of second-person clusivity ('you' vs. 'you and them') in natural languages is controversial and not well attested (Simon 2005). While clusivity is not a feature of standard English language, it is found in many languages around the world. | Wikipedia, 2022

CLUSTER ANALYSIS

  1. (Methodology) A family of computational methods for identification and graphical display of structure in data when the data are too large either in terms of the number of variables or of the number of objects described, or both, to be readily interpretable by direct inspection. All the members of the family work by partitioning a set of obects in the domain of interest into disjoint subjects in accordance with how relatively similar those objects are in terms of the variables that describe them.
    Example: The objects of interest in a certain set of tables are speakers, and each speaker's phonetic usage is described by a set of variables. Any two speakers' usage will be more or less similar depending on how similar their respective variable values are: if the values are identical then so are the speakers in terms of their usage, and the greater the divergence in values the greater the differences. Cluster analysis of the data in a table groups, say, 24 speakers in accordance with how similar their frequency of usage of 12 phonetic segments is.
     There are various kinds of cluster analysis. One of the most frequently used is hierarchical clustering. | Hermann Moisl, 2015
  2. (Methodology) To construct a purely statistical discrimination model, we started experimenting with a statistical technique known as cluster analysis. Succinctly, cluster analysis classifies a set of observations into two or more mutually exclusive groups based on the combination of interval variables. The purpose of cluster analysis is to discover a system of organizing observations into groups, where members of the group share properties. Cluster analysis classifies unknown groups while discriminant function analysis classifies known groups. A common approach to performing a cluster analysis is to first create a table or matrix of relative similarities or differences between all objects and second to use this information to combine objects into groups. The table of relative similarities is called a proximity or dissimilarity matrix. | Pascual Cantos Gómez, 2002

 

Page Last Modified March 2, 2024

 
B a c k   T o   I n d e x