Sank's Glossary of Linguistics 
Chi-Clausd

CHIASTIC STRUCTURE
(Stylistics) Or, chiastic pattern, or, ring structure. A literary structure used in ancient literatures including epic poetry (Odyssey and Iliad); scripture (the Torah, the Bible), as well as in the texts of other pre-modern cultures' texts. Concepts or ideas are placed in a special symmetric order or pattern in a chiastic structure to emphasize them.
 For example, suppose that the first topic in a text is labeled by A, the second topic is labeled by B and the third topic is labeled by C. If the topics in the text appear in the order ABC ... CBA so that the first concept that comes up is also the last, the second is the second to last, and so on, the text is said to have a chiastic structure. Also, a chiastic structure can be of the form ABBAABB ... ABBA. | Wikipedia, 2011

CHILD-DIRECTED SPEECH
(Acquisition) Or, motherese, or, baby talk. Research on different speech registers, so far mainly the domain of sociolinguistics (Coupland and Giles 1988), has focussed on the linguistic and pragmatic features of speech accommodation. In this context, researchers have emphasised the similarities between speech addressed to small children ("child-directed speech", henceforth CDS) and speech addressed to adult interlocutors with limited cognitive or linguistic capacity (elderspeak and foreigner talk or foreigner-directed speech, henceforth FDS) (Kemper, Ferrell, Harden, Finter-Urczyk, and Billington 1998, Coupland, Giles, and Benn 1986, DePaulo and Coleman 1986). CDS, FDS, and elderspeak share such features as reduced grammatical complexity, lower propositional density, an increase of repetitions, clarifications, simplifications, and elaborations (DePaulo and Coleman 1986, Kemper, Vandeputte, Rice, Cheung, and Gubarchuk 1995). However, one of the main characteristics of CDS is its unique prosody.
 The unique prosodic features of CDS comprise of elevated pitch, a wider pitch range and slower speech rate. They are thought to have evolved in order to manipulate infant attention and arousal (Fernald 1993), in particular when physical distances have to be bridged (Falk 2004). Moreover, there is evidence that the prosodic features of CDS facilitate the task of language learning by providing cues for syntactic constituents (Morgan and Demuth 1996). | Sonja Biersack, Vera Kempe, and Lorna Knapton, 2005

CHOMSKY HIERARCHY

  1. (Syntax) Divides all logically possible patterns into nested regions of complexity (Chomsky 1956). Phonological patterns appear to belong to the regular region in this hierarchy (Kaplan and Kay 1994). Heinz (2010) further subdivides the hierarchy with the claim that phonological patterns inhabit a subregion of the hierarchy within the regular class. | Enes Avcu, 2017
  2. (Formal Syntax) Places regular (or linear) languages as a subset of the context-free languages, which in turn are embedded within the set of context-sensitive languages also finally residing in the set of unrestricted or recursively enumerable languages. By defining syntax as the set of rules that define the spatial relationships between the symbols of a language, various levels of language can be also described as one-dimensional (regular or linear), two-dimensional (context-free), three-dimensional (context sensitive) and multi-dimensional (unrestricted) relationships. From these beginnings, Chomsky might well be described as the "father of formal languages". | John A. Lee, 1995

CHUNK

  1. (Computational) In chunking, we carry out segmentation and labeling of multi-token sequences. The smaller boxes [in a figure not shown] show word-level segmentation and labeling, while the large boxes show higher-level segmentation and labeling. It is these larger pieces that we will call "chunks", and the process of identifying them is called chunking. | Steven Bird, Ewan Klein, and Edward Loper, 2008
  2. (Computational) Or, shallow parsing, or, light parsing. An analysis of a sentence which first identifies constituent parts of sentences (nouns, verbs, adjectives, etc.) and then links them to higher order units that have discrete grammatical meanings (noun groups or phrases, verb groups, etc.). While the most elementary chunking algorithms simply link constituent parts on the basis of elementary search patterns (e.g., as specified by regular expressions), approaches that use machine learning techniques (classifiers, topic modeling, etc.) can take contextual information into account and thus compose chunks in such a way that they better reflect the semantic relations between the basic constituents (Jurasky and Martin 2000). That is, these more advanced methods get around the problem that combinations of elementary constituents can have different higher level meanings depending on the context of the sentence.
     It is a technique widely used in natural language processing. It is similar to the concept of lexical analysis for computer languages. Under the name shallow structure hypothesis, it is also used as an explanation for why second language learners often fail to parse complex sentences correctly (Clahsen and Harald, 2006). | Wikipedia, 2021
  3. (Syntax) E.g., idiom chunks, such as too much in too much has been made of this problem. | ?

CHUNKING
(Computational) A generalization of tagging in which a contiguous sequence of words is assigned a single tag. | Steven Bird, Ewan Klein, and Edward Loper, 2006

CISLOCATIVE
See VENITIVE.

CLADISTICS
(Evolution) From the ancient Greek κλάδος, 'branch'. The hierarchical classification of species based on phylogeny or evolutionary ancestry. The term phylogenetics is often used synonymously with "cladistics". Cladistics is distinguished from other taxonomic systems because it focuses on the evolutionary relationships of species rather than on morphological similarities, which may be convergent, and because it places heavy emphasis on objective, quantitative analysis.
 Cladistics originated in the work of the German entomologist Willi Hennig, who himself referred to it as phylogenetic systematics; the use of the terms "cladistics" and clade was popularized by other researchers. Cladistics originated in the field of biology but in recent years has found application in other disciplines. | Fossil Wiki, undated

CLADOGRAM
(Evolution) Cladistics generates diagrams called "cladograms" that represent the evolutionary tree of life. | New World Encyclopedia, 2017

CLASSIFIER

  1. (Grammar) A term used for a variety of elements whose function can somehow be described as that of assigning linguistic expressions to classes.
     In Chinese (1), unless the noun itself denotes a unit of a measurement (e.g. nián 'year'), it must be preceded by a classifier when it occurs with a numeral or a demonstrative.

    1. one

      CL
      qiú
      ball
      'one ball'
     | Glottopedia, 2014
  2. (Grammar) Or, measure word, or, counter word. Abbreviated CLF (Leipzig Glossing Rules) or CL. A word or affix that accompanies nouns and can be considered to "classify" a noun depending on the type of its referent. Classifiers play an important role in certain languages, especially East Asian languages, including Chinese, Korean, Japanese, and Vietnamese. Classifiers are absent or marginal in European languages. An example of a possible classifier in English is piece in phrases like three pieces of paper.
     In languages that have classifiers, they are often used when the noun is being counted, that is, when it appears with a numeral. In such languages, a phrase such as three people is often required to be expressed as three X (of) people, where X is a classifier appropriate to the noun for 'people'. Classifiers sometimes have other functions too; in Chinese, they are commonly used when a noun is preceded by a demonstrative (word meaning 'this' or 'that'). Chinese classifiers are also commonly called measure words, although some writers make a distinction between the two terms. In American Sign Language, particular classifier handshapes represent a noun's orientation in space.
     There are similarities between classifier systems and noun classes, although there are significant differences. Languages with classifiers may have hundreds of classifiers. Languages with noun classes (or in particular, genders) tend to have a smaller number of classifiers. Noun classes are not always dependent on the nouns' meaning but they have a variety of grammatical consequences. | Wikipedia, 2023

CLAUSAL ARGUMENT
(Syntax) A complex puzzle in syntactic analysis is the distribution of "clausal arguments", and in particular of those which can be preceded by an overt pronoun or determiner (generally a definite article, a demonstrative pronoun or a personal pronoun). From a cross-linguistic perspective, the presence of determiners introducing clauses is well attested (e.g. Roussou 1991, Hartman 2012, Kim and Sag 2005, Delicado Cantero 2013, Pietraszko 2019, Jahromi 2011, De Cuba and Ürögdi 2010, among many others). A typical example of this phenomenon is pronoun it in English, which is able to surface before clausal subjects (1) and clausal direct objects (2):

  1. It is important [that you send this document as soon as possible].
  2. I heard it [that Sandra moved out].
 The examples above also have a pronounless counterpart, as shown in (3 and 4):
  1. [That you send this document as soon as possible] is important.
  2. I heard [that Sandra moved out].
 | Mirko Garofalo, 2020

CLAUSAL ARGUMENT ELLIPSIS
(Syntax) It has been observed that argument ellipsis can target not only nominal arguments but also clausal arguments (Shinohara 2006, Saito 2007, Tanaka 2008, Takita 2010). The example in (1) contains a missing embedded clause, which takes the embedded clause of (1b) as its antecedent.

  1. Japanese
    a.
    Mary-wa
    Mary-TOP
    [CP
    zibun-no
    self-GEN
    iken-ga
    idea-NOM
    tadasii
    correct
    to]
    C
    omotteiru.
    think
      'lit. Mary1 thinks [that self1's idea is correct].'
    b.
    John-wa
    John-TOP
    Δ
    omotteinai.
    not.think
      'lit. John2 does not think Δ.' (Δ = [that self1/2's idea is correct])
 The fact that the sloppy reading is available for (1b), where the missing anaphor zibun 'self' refers to John, has been taken as an indication that argument ellipsis can target clausal arguments. Let us call the analysis of null clausal arguments in terms of argument ellipsis the "clausal argument ellipsis" (CAE) analysis. | Kensuka Takita, 2016

CLAUSAL ELLIPSIS
(Syntax) Involves the omission of a whole clause in which a single constituent is left as remnant (Van Craenenbroeck and Merchant 2013). "Clausal ellipsis" is a general expression that covers different subcategories of this type of ellipsis. Two categories are:

  1. Sluicing: Clausal deletion of a complete wh-interrogative clause in which the only element that survives the reduction is the wh-expression (Vicente 2019).
  2. Fragment answers: Utterances answering a question in which nearly all the elements that contribute to a clause are elided, leaving a single constituent as the answer to the question (Gengel 2007). They are constituents which replace the wh-word of the preceding question. These utterances can be produced to make assertions and can be identified as true or false, in other words, they are usually employed to convey propositional meaning (Hall 2019).
 | Sara Expósito Gutiérrez, 2021

CLAUSAL ELLIPTICAL CONSTRUCTION
(Syntax) Fragment answers represent 67.50% of the total number of "clausal elliptical constructions" in Brendan Behan's 1964 play The Hostage. It is the most common subvariety of clausal ellipsis contributing to the cohesion of the dramatic work. From 40 occurrences of clausal ellipsis, 27 correspond to fragment answers such as the ones in (1) and (2).

  1. "Who the hell was that?" – "<That was> My mother." (p. 115)
  2. "What language is she talking?" – "<She's talking> Italian." (p. 161)
 The interrogative clauses preceding the fragment answers illustrated in (1) and (2) are non-elliptical constructions to which the clausal elliptical constructions point anaphorically.
 The only clausal elliptical construction occurring in the narrative part of James Joyce's 1914 composite novel Dubliners is shown in (3).
  1. Why did they never play the grand old operas now, he asked, Dinorah, Lucrezia Borgia? Because they could not get the voices to sing them: that was why <they never played the grand old operas now Dinorah, Lucrezia Borgia>. (p. 143)
 Fragment answers in Dubliners consist of 32 occurrences similar to the ones shown in (4) and (5), representing 72.72% of the total number of clausal ellipses. It is the most common clausal elliptical construction in this literary work.
  1. "What age is he?" – "<He is> nineteen." (p. 79)
  2. "Who's playing up there?" – "Nobody <is playing up there>." (p. 186)
 Fragment answers are the most common occurrences of clausal elliptical constructions in both literary works, contributing to a concise and natural dialogical interaction in both works. | Sara Expósito Gutiérrez, 2021

CLAUSAL LEFT PERIPHERY

  1. (Syntax) On the basis of empirical evidence drawn mainly from Italian, French and English, Rizzi (1997) developed a first articulated structure of the clausal left periphery along the lines of (1).
    1. [ForceP [TopP [FocP [TopP [FinP TP] ] ] ] ]
     In addition to the Force projection, ForceP, which encodes illocutionary force and the Finiteness projection, FinP, which encodes finiteness, there are two projections, TopP and FocP, which are associated with specific discourse functions and are only present when these functions are activated. Topic projections can be iterated, accounting for multiple instances of Clitic Left Dislocation in Romance. As for FocP, the hypothesis is that it can only be activated once in a single C field. Rizzi (1997) accounts for the uniqueness of FocP in terms of interpretation while Haegeman (2012) provides an explanation based on intervention. In later work, Rizzi has argued for additional projections, including ModP (Rizzi 2014) for left peripheral adjuncts, as well as IntP (Rizzi 2001) activated in polar questions. | Marc Authier and Liliane Haegeman, 2018
  2. (Syntax) Following Rizzi (1997), the CP domain at the left edge of the clause consists of multiple functional projections (or series thereof). These projections host diverse phrase-initial XPs that are in some sense peripheral to rather than part and parcel of the clause as traditionally defined. For Old French, a similar idea appeared in Skårup (1975), whose model of the Medieval Romance clause appears in (1). Following Diderichsen's positional syntax (e.g., 1957), Skårup posits a series of zones: the verbal zone hosts the finite verb and corresponds descriptively to the second position, and the preverbal zone corresponds to the first position. Importantly, the preverbal zone is preceded by the extraposition zone, which could host XPs that Skårup considered to be outside the clause proper ("hors de la proposition") but nonetheless attached to the following clause.
    1. [Extraposition] [Preverbal zone (=fondement)] [VERBAL ZONE] [Postverbal zone]
     | Bryan Donaldson, 2012

See Also PERIPHERY.

CLAUSAL PRO-FORM
(Grammar) German is known for having different constructions with a pronominal element relating to a sentence-internal dependent clause; cf. (1-5). The term "clausal pro-form" will be used to refer to such pronominal elements.

  1. Maria
    Maria
    hat
    has
    es
    it
    stets
    always
    bedauert,
    regretted
    dass
    that
    Peter
    Peter
    berühmt
    famous
    ist.
    is
    'Maria has always regretted that Peter is famous.'
  2. Maria
    Maria
    hat
    has
    es
    it
    behauptet,
    claimed
    dass
    that
    Peter
    Peter
    berühmt
    famous
    ist.
    is
    'Maria claimed that Peter is famous.'
  3. Maria
    Maria
    hat
    has
    sich
    REFL
    darüber
    about-that
    gewundert,
    wondered
    dass
    that
    Peter
    Peter
    berühmt
    famous
    ist.
    is
    'Maria was surprised that Peter is famous.'
  4. Maria
    Maria
    ist
    is
    darum
    therefore
    der
    the
    Einladung
    invitation
    gefolgt,
    followed
    weil
    because
    der
    the
    Gastgeber
    host
    berühmt
    famous
    ist.
    is
    'Maria accepted the invitation because the host is famous.'
  5. Maria
    Maria.ACC
    hat
    has
    es
    it
    überrascht,
    surprised
    dass
    that
    Peter
    Peter
    berühmt
    famous
    ist.
    is
    'It surprised Maria that Peter is famous.'
 | Werner Frey, 2016

CLAUSAL SUBJECT
(Grammar) A clausal syntactic subject of a clause; i.e., the subject is itself a clause. The governor of this relation might not always be a verb: when the verb is a copular verb, the root of the clause is the complement of the copular verb. The dependent is the main lexical verb or other predicate of the subject clause. In the following examples, what she said (that is, said) is the clausal subject of makes and interesting, respectively.

  1. What she said makes sense.
  2. What she said is interesting.
  3. What she said was well received.
 | Universal Dependencies

 

Page Halved By Split February 14, 2024

 
B a c k   T o   I n d e x